Write For Us

How can understanding motion perception improve vision?

E-Commerce Solutions SEO Solutions Marketing Solutions
171 Views
Published
Visual motion is an important source of information for separating objects from their backgrounds.

A spider camouflaged against a branch, for instance, immediately loses its invisibility once it starts moving. A friend you’re trying to spot in a crowded airport terminal is more distinguishable once she begins waving her hands.

According to new research from scientists at the University of Rochester, one reason human beings are good at discerning smaller moving objects in the foreground is that the brain becomes desensitized to larger background stimuli. Conversely, when a person’s brain is more sensitive to background motion, the negative trade-off is that she will be less sensitive to smaller foreground objects. The research, published in the journal Nature Communications, could lead to new training programs for elderly adults and patients with conditions such as schizophrenia and depression, which have been linked to weaker motion segregation.

“The human brain cannot possibly process all of the information around us,” says Duje Tadin, a professor of brain and cognitive sciences at Rochester and the lead author of the study. “Being less sensitive to things that are less important makes the brain more efficient and faster at accomplishing the more important tasks.”

Take driving, for instance. As the background scenery whizzes by, it is imperative that a driver see and avoid cars, pedestrians, and other objects on the road.

There are two basic ways the brain can distinguish such objects from moving backgrounds. It can enhance the objects that matter; or, it can suppress the background, and, by virtue of this suppression, enhance the objects. The latter is “the more efficient option,” Tadin says. “Think about trying to have a conversation in a room with high background noise. It is more effective to find a way to turn off the noise than it is to just try speaking more loudly.”

In order to test people’s ability to identify moving objects on a moving background, the researchers showed study participants moving textured patterns. Within the textured background, there was a smaller patterned object moving in the direction opposite from the background. The participants were instructed to report either the location or the shape of the smaller patterned object.

The researchers found that younger adults were better at seeing smaller moving objects in the foreground, and worse at seeing background motion. Older adults—participants aged 65 and above—were the opposite. They were poorer at seeing the smaller moving objects because they had a heightened awareness of the backdrop against which the objects moved. Younger adults took on average 20 milliseconds to pick out the moving objects, and older adults took about 30 milliseconds.

While both groups were efficient at the task, taking only a fraction of a second to detect the movement of the object against the background, “those extra milliseconds could make a big difference,” says Woon Ju Park, a former postdoctoral associate in Tadin’s lab and currently a research associate at the University of Washington. A split second could mean the difference between hitting or avoiding a pedestrian; or be just enough time to lose sight of a rambunctious child. In the case of the animal world, it could mean the difference between life and death. In evolutionary terms, being able to quickly detect motion is critical for a species’ survival.

“If you think of an animal in the woods, if there is a moving object, that could either be lunch for the animal or something that could eat that animal for lunch,” Park says. “Animals are really good at camouflage, but even the best camouflage pulls apart with motion.”

Although the research shows that the ability to detect moving objects against a moving background decreases with age, the research also offers some good news for older adults.

The researchers found that older adults could train their brains to process motion more like younger adults by practicing visual segmentation of moving objects. Older participants performed the study task for four weeks, with four sessions per week, and became quicker at the task, narrowing the gap in performance with their younger counterparts. Surprisingly, the researchers found, the older participants who underwent training did not, in fact, get better at seeing the smaller moving object; their ability to see the target was just as good as it was at the beginning of the training. What changed with training was that the older adults became less sensitive to the background motion, just like younger adults.

Help us caption & translate this video!

https://amara.org/v/BBTEP/
Category
Academic
Sign in or sign up to post comments.
Be the first to comment